Home About us Editorial board Ahead of print Current issue Search Archives Submit article Instructions Subscribe Contacts Login 
5 years IF: 1.036 (® Thomson Reuters)
IF 2016: 1.064 (® Thomson Reuters)
Total Cites: 7140
Follow Us
Follow Us
  • Users Online: 2163
  • Home
  • Print this page
  • Email this page

 Table of Contents  
ORIGINAL ARTICLE
Year : 2017  |  Volume : 130  |  Issue : 13  |  Page : 1513-1520

Effect of an Ecological Executive Skill Training Program for School-aged Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Clinical Trial


1 Clinical Psychological Center, Peking University Sixth Hospital (Institute of Mental Health), National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health (Peking University), Beijing 100191, China
2 Child Psychiatric Research Center, Peking University Sixth Hospital (Institute of Mental Health), National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health (Peking University), Beijing 100191, China

Date of Submission09-Feb-2017
Date of Web Publication16-Jun-2017

Correspondence Address:
Li Yang
Child Psychiatric Research Center, Peking University Sixth Hospital (Institute of Mental Health), National Clinical Research Center for Mental Disorders, Key Laboratory of Mental Health, Ministry of Health (Peking University), Beijing 100191
China
Login to access the Email id

Source of Support: None, Conflict of Interest: None


DOI: 10.4103/0366-6999.208236

Rights and Permissions
  Abstract 


Background: As medication does not normalize outcomes of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), especially in real-life functioning, nonpharmacological methods are important to target this field. This randomized controlled clinical trial was designed to evaluate the effects of a comprehensive executive skill training program for school-aged children with ADHD in a relatively large sample.
Methods: The children (aged 6–12 years) with ADHD were randomized to the intervention or waitlist groups. A healthy control group was composed of gender- and age-matched healthy children. The intervention group received a 12-session training program for multiple executive skills. Executive function (EF), ADHD symptoms, and social functioning in the intervention and waitlist groups were evaluated at baseline and the end of the final training session. The healthy controls (HCs) were only assessed once at baseline. Repeated measures analyses of variance were used to compare EF, ADHD symptoms, and social function between intervention and waitlist groups.
Results: Thirty-eight children with ADHD in intervention group, 30 in waitlist group, and 23 healthy children in healthy control group were included in final analysis. At posttreatment, intervention group showed significantly lower Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) total score (135.89 ± 16.80 vs. 146.09 ± 23.92, P= 0.04) and monitoring score (18.05 ± 2.67 vs. 19.77 ± 3.10, P= 0.02), ADHD-IV overall score (41.11 ± 7.48 vs. 47.20 ± 8.47, P< 0.01), hyperactivity-impulsivity (HI) subscale score (18.92 ± 5.09 vs. 21.93 ± 4.93, P= 0.02), and inattentive subscale score (22.18 ± 3.56 vs. 25.27 ± 5.06, P< 0.01), compared with the waitlist group. Repeated measures analyses of variance revealed significant interactions between time and group on the BRIEF inhibition subscale (F = 5.06, P= 0.03), working memory (F = 4.48, P= 0.04), ADHD-IV overall score (F = 21.72, P< 0.01), HI subscale score (F = 19.08, P< 0.01), and inattentive subscale score (F = 12.40, P< 0.01). Multiple-way analysis of variance showed significant differences on all variables of BRIEF, ADHD-rating scale-IV, and WEISS Functional Impairment Scale-Parent form (WFIRS-P) among the intervention and waitlist groups at posttreatment and HCs at baseline.
Conclusions: This randomized controlled study on executive skill training in a relatively large sample provided some evidences that the training could improve EF deficits, reduce problematic symptoms, and potentially enhance the social functioning in school-aged children with ADHD.
Clinical Trial Registration: http://www.clinicaltrials.gov; NCT02327585.

Keywords: Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder; Executive Function; Executive Skill Training; Randomized Controlled Trial


How to cite this article:
Qian Y, Chen M, Shuai L, Cao QJ, Yang L, Wang YF. Effect of an Ecological Executive Skill Training Program for School-aged Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Clinical Trial. Chin Med J 2017;130:1513-20

How to cite this URL:
Qian Y, Chen M, Shuai L, Cao QJ, Yang L, Wang YF. Effect of an Ecological Executive Skill Training Program for School-aged Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Randomized Controlled Clinical Trial. Chin Med J [serial online] 2017 [cited 2017 Oct 17];130:1513-20. Available from: http://www.cmj.org/text.asp?2017/130/13/1513/208236

Ying Qian and Min Chen contributed equally to this work.





  Introduction Top


Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is now increasingly recognized as a developmental impairment that involves deficient executive function (EF).[1],[2] The term “executive function” refers to a set of regulatory processes that are needed to select, initiate, implement, and oversee thought, emotion, behavior, and certain facets of motor and sensory functions.[3] It comprises several domains, such as inhibition, initiation, sustaining attention, set shifting, working memory, emotional regulation, planning, organizing, and monitoring.[4] Studies have indicated that children with ADHD who experienced executive dysfunction were at a higher risk for academic failure and poor psychosocial and occupational outcomes than those who had intact EF.[5],[6]

Although substantial literature has demonstrated the efficacy of medication on improving EF in ADHD through short/medium-term therapy, medications have several potential limitations including partial response or nonresponse,[7] possible side effects,[8] uncertainty about the long-term costs and benefits,[9] and poor adherence.[10] Moreover, except sustained attention, evidence showed that medications did not necessarily normalize the neuropsychological outcomes of children with ADHD.[11] For these reasons, pharmacological treatment alone might not be sufficient to remediate the deficits associated with ADHD, and it is thus necessary to develop additional treatment methods that target core neuropsychological deficits.[12],[13]

EF training has been investigated as a potential treatment method for ADHD that might have more long-term benefits and fewer side effects than pharmacological treatment. It is hypothesized that EF training might reduce ADHD symptoms and improve functioning by targeting neuropsychological deficits to mediate ADHD pathophysiology. There are two types of EF training: facilitative intervention training (FIT), which fosters the development of EF through the use of computer-based training programs,[14],[15],[16],[17],[18],[19],[20] and compensatory EF training, which is based on behavioral therapy. Regarding FIT, although some evidence indicated that training working memory could improve working memory measures, no significant improvements were found for attention and/or mixed EF training in a meta-analysis.[14] The effects on working memory did not transfer to other neuropsychological tests, behavior, or academic function,[14] which suggested the need to develop ecologically effective EF training programs. The compensatory EF training based on behavioral therapy might fit such need.

Some studies have discussed the effectiveness of executive skill training programs that are designed to enhance EF in children's daily lives,[21],[22],[23],[24],[25] three of which were open trials, and two were randomized waitlist control trials. Almost all of them reported significant improvement in ADHD symptoms rated by parents, teachers, and a blinded clinician. Some also reported significant differences between treated and untreated patients in neuropsychological measures including visual-spatial memory, planning, visual/auditory attention, and cognitive flexibility, corresponding to the improvements in parent ratings of ecological executive functioning. Notably, Abikoff et al.[25] compared skill training with a performance-based intervention and found that the former improved academic performance and proficiency to a greater extent than the latter. These previous studies suggested that executive skill training might be an ecologically valid treatment that deserves development and validation in a larger sample. Furthermore, most previous studies were performed in children younger than 7 years old. As the period from 6 to 12 years of age represents a period of “pruning” synaptic connections, and this pruning process helps consolidate learning skills, this period might be a good time to train skills in school-aged children.

We thus hypothesized that intervention targeting executive dysfunction in everyday life might relieve the EF deficits of children with ADHD, and the core symptoms of ADHD and social dysfunction might be improved with the relief of executive dysfunction. This randomized controlled clinical trial was designed to evaluate the effects of a comprehensive executive skill training program for school-age children with ADHD in a relatively large sample. This program was to target the putative dysfunction underlying ADHD and promote the development of EF. The effects of the training were systemically evaluated using neuropsychological tests and behavioral ratings of EF, symptoms of ADHD, and social function.


  Methods Top


Ethical approval

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Peking University Sixth Hospital. Parents and children signed separate informed written consent before their enrollment in this study.

Participants

All participants were children with ADHD recruited from a mental health hospital in Beijing. The diagnosis of ADHD was first made according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV (DSM-IV) criteria based on parent ratings of the ADHD-rating scale-IV (ADHD-RS-IV)[26] and was then confirmed by a semi-structured interview conducted by experienced pediatric psychiatrists using the clinical diagnostic interview scale.[27] Children were excluded if they met the following criteria: a history of head injury, a diagnosis of other congenital or acquired neurological conditions, an estimated full-scale IQ <80, a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder, psychosis, or an emergent psychiatric condition that needed immediate medication. Ten participants had maintained steady dosage of medications for more than half a year and remained unchanged during the entire study. New medications could not be initiated during the study. The participants aged from 6 years to 12 years and were randomized to the intervention or waitlist groups. A healthy control group was composed of gender- and age-matched healthy children from a primary school nearby.

Study design and procedure

The study adopted a randomized block design to balance the individuals between the intervention and waitlist groups. Children with ADHD who met the eligibility criteria were randomized to a block that comprised a permutation of four participants, two for each group separately. The grouping scheme was concealed in envelopes, and the recruited participant was notified of his or her group sequentially. During the study, the intervention group received a 12-session training program for multiple executive skills once a week, whereas the waitlist group did not receive any new intervention. After the postintervention data collection, participants in the waitlist group had the opportunity to receive the same intervention to adhere to ethical considerations.

Executive skill training

The training program, based on Dawson and Guare's (2010) training of executive skills for children, aimed to improve the everyday performance of executive skills among children with ADHD. We added some culturally adapted information, following the theory of cognitive behavioral therapy, to ensure that the concepts were acceptable to Chinese children.

The training program included 12 weekly 1-h sessions in a clinical setting and was applied in groups of 6 to 8 families. Regarding the parents' involvement, all parents received the first and last sessions and were instructed to help their children finish the homework outlined in the handbook between sessions. As for the children, all children attended the other ten sessions and were asked to complete the homework in the handbook between sessions with the help of their parents. Further details are described in [Supplementary Material 1] [Additional file 1] .

The intervention was provided by a senior pediatric psychiatrist and a trained graduate student. All the training principles and processes described above were documented in a manual for the trainer.

Outcome measurement

EF, ADHD symptoms, and social functioning in the intervention and waitlist groups were evaluated at baseline and the end of the final training session. The healthy controls (HCs) were only assessed once at baseline.

For EF measurement, performance-based tasks were selected from the Chinese neuropsychological battery used by our research group.[28] This battery includes the Stroop Task, Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Test, and Trail-making Test [Supplementary Material 2] [Additional file 2] . The interrater reliability kappa coefficients of the tests ranged from 0.70 to 0.75 (P < 0.01).

The Behavior Rating Inventory of Executive Function (BRIEF) parent form is a questionnaire that assesses EF behaviors in daily life environments.[29] The instrument includes eight subscales: initiation, working memory, planning, organization of materials, monitoring, inhibition, shifting, and emotional control. This scale has been translated into Chinese and has good psychometric properties and clinical discrimination, with a test-retest reliability of 0.68–0.89 and a Cronbach's coefficient of 0.74–0.96.[30]

For ADHD symptom measurement, the ADHD-RS-IV [26] was used to assess symptoms. This scale consists of 18 items corresponding to the DSM-IV criteria for ADHD. The overall symptom scores as well as the inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity (HI) scores were calculated based on different symptom domains. This scale was translated into Chinese by Su et al.,[31] with a test-retest reliability of 0.72 and a Cronbach's coefficient of 0.91.

The WEISS Functional Impairment Scale-Parent form (WFIRS-P) used for social function measurement is a questionnaire including six subscales: assessing family, learning and school, life skills, self-concept, social activities, and risky activities. This scale was implemented in China by Qian et al.,[32] with a test-retest reliability of 0.61–0.87 and a Cronbach's coefficient of 0.70–0.92.

Statistical analysis

In this study, group differences in demographic and initial clinical variables of ADHD were assessed using Student's t-test for quantitative data and Chi-square test for categorical data. The 2 × 2 repeated measures analyses were used to evaluate the effects of time (pre- and post-treatment), group (intervention vs. waitlist), and the group-time interaction on EF, ADHD symptoms, and social function, with group as a between-subject factor and time as a within-subject factor. The neuropsychological, behavioral, and functional measurements of the intervention and waitlist groups at the end of training were compared to those of the HCs using multiple-way analysis of variance (MANOVA) with gender, age, and IQ as covariates to determine whether the training normalized the deficits of ADHD. The measurement data were shown as mean ± standard deviation (SD). Statistical analyses were performed using SPSS version 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). All statistical tests were two-tailed, and a P< 0.05 was considered statistically significant.


  Results Top


A total of 86 children with ADHD were randomized to the intervention (n = 44) or waitlist groups (n = 42). Sixty-eight children (79.1%) completed the whole study; 18 children dropped out due to scheduling problems and time conflicts, six of whom were in the intervention group (13.6%) and 12 in the waitlist group (28.6%). The 86.4% (38/44) of children in intervention group complied with the training, completing ten or more sessions in the 12-session period. All of the missed group sessions were administered to the trainee individually. Thirty-eight children in intervention group (including 32 boys and 6 girls) and thirty in waitlist group (including 22 boys and 8 girls) were included in final analysis. The demographic and clinical characteristics of these participants are presented in [Table 1]. For intervention group, the mean age was 8.3 ± 1.3 years, and the mean IQ was 105.7 ± 13.9; for waitlist group, the mean age was 7.8 ± 1.2 years, and the mean IQ was 101.8 ± 10.4. There were no significant differences in age, gender, IQ, ADHD subtypes, and comorbidity between two groups at baseline (all P> 0.05). At baseline, no significant differences were also found in EF, ADHD symptoms, and social function between intervention and waitlist groups (all P> 0.05).
Table 1: Demographic and clinical characteristics of the children with ADHD in this study

Click here to view


Twenty-three gender- and age-matched children without ADHD (including 15 boys and 8 girls) voluntarily participated in the study as HCs. The HCs completed the same cognitive function test battery at baseline. The mean age of HCs was 8.4 ± 0.9 years, and the mean IQ was 108.6 ± 12.2.

At posttreatment, intervention group showed significantly lower BRIEF total score (135.89 ± 16.80 vs. 146.09 ± 23.92, P= 0.04) and monitoring score (18.05 ± 2.67 vs. 19.77 ± 3.10, P= 0.02), ADHD-IV overall score (41.11 ± 7.48 vs. 47.20 ± 8.47, P< 0.01), HI subscale score (18.92 ± 5.09 vs. 21.93 ± 4.93, P= 0.02), and inattentive subscale score (22.18 ± 3.56 vs. 25.27 ± 5.06, P< 0.01), compared with the waitlist group. The inhibition factor on BRIEF (17.13 ± 3.77 vs. 19.17 ± 4.84, P= 0.05) and learning/school subscale of the WFIRS-P (6.32 ± 3.63 vs. 8.03 ± 3.77, P= 0.06) in the intervention group also showed a decreased trend, compared with the waitlist group.

Repeated measures analyses of variance revealed significant interactions between time and group on the BRIEF inhibition subscale (F = 5.06, P= 0.03), working memory (F = 4.48, P= 0.04; [Table 2]), ADHD-IV overall score (F = 21.72, P< 0.01), HI subscale score (F = 19.08, P< 0.01), and inattentive subscale score (F = 12.40, P< 0.01; [Table 3]). Trend significance was also observed for the BRIEF total score (F = 3.89, P= 0.05) and monitoring subscale (F = 3.91, P= 0.05), the forgotten structure score on the ROCF (F = 4.27, P= 0.05), risky activities subscale (F = 3.78, P= 0.06), and learning/school subscale (F = 4.03, P= 0.05) of the WFIRS-P [Table 4].
Table 2: Repeated measures analyses of variance in executive function among the intervention and waitlist groups at baseline and posttreatment

Click here to view
Table 3: Repeated measures analyses of variance in ADHD symptoms among the intervention and waitlist groups at baseline and posttreatment

Click here to view
Table 4: Repeated measures analyses of variance in social functioning among the intervention and waitlist groups at baseline and posttreatment

Click here to view


MANOVA showed significant differences on all variables of BRIEF, ADHD-RS-IV, and WEISS among the intervention and waitlist groups at posttreatment and HCs at baseline [Table 5]. Both intervention and waitlist groups showed significant differences almost on all scales, compared with the HC group.
Table 5: Executive function, ADHD symptoms, and social functioning in posttreatment intervention and waitlist groups and healthy controls

Click here to view



  Discussion Top


The executive skill training program used in this study was developed as an ecological nonpharmacological intervention for children with ADHD. This study provided some evidence that this program improved EF and ADHD symptoms, which potentially promoted social function.

Barkley [33] proposed that inhibition impairment might be a core deficit of ADHD. This inhibition deficit was expressed as impulsivity.[34] In children with ADHD, impulsivity was often manifested as the inability to wait in a variety of situations and as the tendency to interrupt others' conversations or to respond before the end of a question.[35] Children with more prominent HI symptoms showed more risk-taking behaviors.[36],[37]

This randomized waitlist-controlled study found that executive skill training improved the inhibition factor of the BRIEF, HI score of the ADHD-RS-IV, and possibly the risky activities subscale of the WFIRS-P, indicating that the targeted training of inhibitory skills could alleviate inhibition deficits, decrease HI symptoms, and potentially reduce risk-taking behavior in daily life settings. The results of this study were consistent with the possible mechanisms of HI behavior and related functions, which were supported by Barkley's hypothesis.[33]

This study did not find significant differences between the intervention group and the waitlist group on the Stroop test (performance-based test) although significant differences were found on the inhibition subscale of the BRIEF (valid ecological rating). The low agreement between the performance-based and ecological ratings of EF has been reported in other studies as well.[38],[39] An explanation of this result was that neuropsychological tests have less ecological validity in evaluating children's behaviors in everyday life. Behavioral questionnaires for parents and teachers or direct observations of behaviors are probably more sensitive to the adaptive modifications occurring in natural contexts.[22] As the training in this study focused more on ecological executive skills, it might have been difficult to detect the changes on performance-based tasks.

ADHD is acknowledged to be the most common cognitive disorder in childhood. Many cognitive studies have suggested that there are working memory deficits in ADHD children.[40],[41],[42] Inattention and impaired cognitive abilities serve as dual pathways between ADHD and later academic achievement.[43],[44],[45],[46] This study found that the executive skill training improved immediate or delayed recall scores of the ROCF, the working memory subscale of the BRIEF, ADHD-RS-IV inattention score, and possibly the learning and school subscale of the WFIRS-P, which suggested that the executive skill training improved ADHD children's working memory (in laboratory performance as well as in everyday life settings) and attention, both of which contributed to better academic performance. The effects of the training on school and academic performance provided important support for the hypothesis that executive skill training is ecologically valid and potentially leads to the transfer of treatment gains. Completing the homework assigned to the participants in the 12-week training period promoted generalization of the learned skills.

Targeted training for EF deficits adopts two forms: computerized cognitive training and noncomputerized skill training. Sonuga-Barke et al.[47] systematically reviewed the available psychological treatments for ADHD.[44] A meta-analysis revealed that computerized cognitive training only improved working memory in laboratory settings, with a limited transfer of effects on ADHD symptoms and academic functions. For behavioral interventions, although the standardized mean difference was near zero, none of the included studies focused on executive skill training that targeted the core impairment of ADHD. Few studies focused on noncomputerized executive skill training programs. Halperin et al.[21] combined executive, attention, and motor skill training and found that ADHD severity improved significantly as rated by the parent and teacher on the ADHD-RS-IV from pre- to post-treatment. Both inattention and HI domain scores declined. However, it is challenging to disentangle the effects of executive skill training. Miranda et al.[22] analyzed the effects of an intensive psychosocial intervention on EF, which taught children a set of cognitive-behavioral techniques. Improvement was observed in both cognitive function (visual-spatial memory and planning) and ADHD symptoms (HI and inattention). Tamm et al.[23],[24] developed a metacognitive EF training program for young children with ADHD. Open as well as randomized waitlist-controlled trials showed that the training had effects on both laboratory performance and parent-rated EF. Thus, high consistency was found between this study and previous studies, and we further assessed functional outcomes. All of the findings described above strongly indicate that executive skill training is a potentially effective nonpharmacological intervention for EF, ADHD symptoms, and school performance.

The results of this study indicated that the intervention group did not catch up with the HCs on almost all variables at the end of the study. Thus, although the executive skill training provided in this study was shown to be an effective nonpharmacological treatment for school-aged children with ADHD, it still needs to be modified or combined with other types of therapy to help children reach full recovery.

There were some limitations in this study. This study used nonblind parent ratings as the primary outcome measure, which might have been influenced by the subjective expectations of the parents. However, the changes observed in neuropsychology, behavior, and social function, which were consistent with previous studies, potentially corresponded to the rational of the training. In addition, the improvement was not pervasive, which decreased the possibility of subjective bias. Another limitation was the trend toward significant improvement in social function, which might have been due to insufficient training intensity or the short follow-up time. In the future, these limitations can be addressed by revising the protocol to increase the training intensity, adding more assessment measures, and focusing on subgroups of ADHD patients (e.g., ADHD with mild impairment).

In summary, this study supported the hypothesis that executive skill training could improve EF deficits, reduce problematic symptoms, and consequently improve the social function of school-aged children with ADHD. However, the improvements were still insufficient to normalize the symptoms of the participants.

Supplementary information is linked to the online version of the paper on the Chinese Medical Journal website.

Financial support and sponsorship

The study was supported by grants from the Beijing Municipal Science and Technology Commission (No. Z151100004015103), the Major State Basic Research Development Program of China (973 Program, No. 2014CB846100), National Key Research Plan of Ministry of Science and Technology of China (No. 2016YFC1306103), and the Capital Health Development Research Fund (No. 2011-4024-04).

Conflicts of interest

There are no conflicts of interest.



 
  References Top

1.
Brown TE. ADD/ADHD and impaired executive function in clinical practice. Curr Psychiatry Rep 2008;10:407-11.  Back to cited text no. 1
[PUBMED]    
2.
Brown TE, Holdnack J, Saylor K, Adler L, Spencer T, Williams DW, et al. Effect of atomoxetine on executive function impairments in adults with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2011;15:130-8. doi: 10.1177/1087054709356165.  Back to cited text no. 2
[PUBMED]    
3.
Isquith PK, Roth RM, Gioia G. Contribution of rating scales to the assessment of executive functions. Appl Neuropsychol Child 2013;2:125-32. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2013.748389.  Back to cited text no. 3
[PUBMED]    
4.
Castellanos FX, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Milham MP, Tannock R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci 2006;10:117-23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011.  Back to cited text no. 4
[PUBMED]    
5.
Biederman J, Monuteaux MC, Mick E, Spencer T, Wilens TE, Silva JM, et al. Young adult outcome of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: A controlled 10-year follow-up study. Psychol Med 2006;36:167-79. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705006410.  Back to cited text no. 5
[PUBMED]    
6.
Zheng Y, Liang JM, Gao HY, Yang ZW, Jia FJ, Liang YZ, et al. An Open-label, Self-control, Prospective Study on Cognitive Function, Academic Performance, and Tolerability of Osmotic-release Oral System Methylphenidate in Children with Attention-deficit Hyperactivity Disorder. Chin Med J 2015;128:2988-97. doi: 10.4103/0366-6999.168948.  Back to cited text no. 6
[PUBMED]  [Full text]  
7.
Faraone SV, Biederman J, Spencer TJ, Aleardi M. Comparing the efficacy of medications for ADHD using meta-analysis. MedGenMed 2006;8:4.  Back to cited text no. 7
    
8.
Cortese S, Holtmann M, Banaschewski T, Buitelaar J, Coghill D, Danckaerts M, et al. Practitioner review: Current best practice in the management of adverse events during treatment with ADHD medications in children and adolescents. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013;54:227-46. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12036.  Back to cited text no. 8
[PUBMED]    
9.
Molina BS, Hinshaw SP, Swanson JM, Arnold LE, Vitiello B, Jensen PS, et al. The MTA at 8 years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2009;48:484-500. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0.  Back to cited text no. 9
[PUBMED]    
10.
Adler LD, Nierenberg AA. Review of medication adherence in children and adults with ADHD. Postgrad Med 2010;122:184-91. doi: 10.3810/pgm.2010.01.2112.  Back to cited text no. 10
[PUBMED]    
11.
Advokat C. What are the cognitive effects of stimulant medications? Emphasis on adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2010;34:1256-66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.03.006.  Back to cited text no. 11
[PUBMED]    
12.
Rabipour S, Raz A. Training the brain: Fact and fad in cognitive and behavioral remediation. Brain Cogn 2012;79:159-79. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.02.006.  Back to cited text no. 12
[PUBMED]    
13.
Solanto MV, Marks DJ, Mitchell KJ, Wasserstein J, Kofman MD. Development of a new psychosocial treatment for adult ADHD. J Atten Disord 2008;11:728-36. doi: 10.1177/1087054707305100.  Back to cited text no. 13
[PUBMED]    
14.
Rapport MD, Orban SA, Kofler MJ, Friedman LM. Do programs designed to train working memory, other executive functions, and attention benefit children with ADHD? A meta-analytic review of cognitive, academic, and behavioral outcomes. Clin Psychol Rev 2013;33:1237-52. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.08.005.  Back to cited text no. 14
[PUBMED]    
15.
Beck SJ, Hanson CA, Puffenberger SS, Benninger KL, Benninger WB. A controlled trial of working memory training for children and adolescents with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2010;39:825-36. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2010.517162.  Back to cited text no. 15
[PUBMED]    
16.
Chacko A, Feirsen N, Bedard AC, Marks D, Uderman JZ, Chimiklis A. Cogmed Working Memory Training for youth with ADHD: A closer examination of efficacy utilizing evidence-based criteria. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol 2013;42:769-83. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.787622.  Back to cited text no. 16
[PUBMED]    
17.
Chacko A, Bedard AC, Marks DJ, Feirsen N, Uderman JZ, Chimiklis A, et al. A randomized clinical trial of Cogmed Working Memory Training in school-age children with ADHD: A replication in a diverse sample using a control condition. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2014;55:247-55. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12146.  Back to cited text no. 17
[PUBMED]    
18.
Klingberg T, Forssberg H, Westerberg H. Training of working memory in children with ADHD. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 2002;24:781-91. doi: 10.1076/jcen.24.6.781.8395.  Back to cited text no. 18
[PUBMED]    
19.
Klingberg T, Fernell E, Olesen PJ, Johnson M, Gustafsson P, Dahlström K, et al. Computerized training of working memory in children with ADHD – A randomized, controlled trial. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2005;44:177-86. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200502000-00010.  Back to cited text no. 19
[PUBMED]    
20.
Green CT, Long DL, Green D, Iosif AM, Dixon JF, Miller MR, et al. Will working memory training generalize to improve off-task behavior in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Neurotherapeutics 2012;9:639-48. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0124-y.  Back to cited text no. 20
    
21.
Halperin JM, Marks DJ, Bedard AC, Chacko A, Curchack JT, Yoon CA, et al. Training executive, attention, and motor skills: A proof-of-concept study in preschool children with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2013;17:711-21. doi: 10.1177/1087054711435681.  Back to cited text no. 21
    
22.
Miranda A, Presentación MJ, Siegenthaler R, Jara P. Effects of a psychosocial intervention on the executive functioning in children with ADHD. J Learn Disabil 2013;46:363-76. doi: 10.1177/0022219411427349.  Back to cited text no. 22
    
23.
Tamm L, Nakonezny PA, Hughes CW. An open trial of a metacognitive executive function training for young children with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2014;18:551-9. doi: 10.1177/1087054712445782.  Back to cited text no. 23
    
24.
Tamm L, Nakonezny PA. Metacognitive executive function training for young children with ADHD: A proof-of-concept study. Atten Defic Hyperact Disord 2015;7:183-90. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0162-x.  Back to cited text no. 24
    
25.
Abikoff H, Gallagher R, Wells KC, Murray DW, Huang L, Lu F, et al. Remediating organizational functioning in children with ADHD: Immediate and long-term effects from a randomized controlled trial. J Consult Clin Psychol 2013;81:113-28. doi: 10.1007/s12402-014-0162-x.  Back to cited text no. 25
    
26.
DuPaul GJ. ADHD Rating Scale-IV: Checklists, Norms, and Clinical Interpretation. New York: Guilford Press; 1998.  Back to cited text no. 26
    
27.
Barkley RA. Attention-Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press; 1998.  Back to cited text no. 27
    
28.
Qian Y, Shuai L, Cao Q, Chan RC, Wang Y. Do executive function deficits differentiate between children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and ADHD comorbid with oppositional defiant disorder? A cross-cultural study using performance-based tests and the behavior rating inventory of executive function. Clin Neuropsychol 2010;24:793-810. doi: 10.1080/13854041003749342.  Back to cited text no. 28
    
29.
Gioia GA, Isquith PK, Kenworthy L, Barton RM. Profiles of everyday executive function in acquired and developmental disorders. Child Neuropsychol 2002;8:121-37. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.2.121.8727.  Back to cited text no. 29
    
30.
Qian Y, Wang YF. Reliability and validity of behavior rating scale of executive function parent form for school age children in China (in Chinese). J Peking Univ Health Sci 2007;39:277-83.  Back to cited text no. 30
    
31.
Su LY, Geng YG, Wang H, Du YS, Sun L. Norm of ADHD rating scale – Parent version in Chinese urban children (in Chinese). Chin J Prac Pediatr 2006;21:833-6.  Back to cited text no. 31
    
32.
Qian Y, Du QX, Qu S, Wang YF. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of Weiss Functional Impairment Scale – Parent form for school age children. Chin Ment Health J 2011;25:767-71.  Back to cited text no. 32
    
33.
Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull 1997;121:65-94.  Back to cited text no. 33
    
34.
Bari A, Robbins TW. Inhibition and impulsivity: Behavioral and neural basis of response control. Prog Neurobiol 2013;108:44-79. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2013.06.005.  Back to cited text no. 34
    
35.
American Psychiatric Association, American Psychiatric Association, DSM-5 Task Force. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association; 2013.  Back to cited text no. 35
    
36.
De Alwis D, Lynskey MT, Reiersen AM, Agrawal A. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes and substance use and use disorders in NESARC. Addict Behav 2014;39:1278-85. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2014.04.003.  Back to cited text no. 36
    
37.
Ortal S, van de Glind G, Johan F, Itai B, Nir Y, Iliyan I, et al. The role of different aspects of impulsivity as independent risk factors for substance use disorders in patients with ADHD: A review. Curr Drug Abuse Rev 2015;8:119-33. doi: 10.2174/1874473708666150916112913.  Back to cited text no. 37
    
38.
Toplak ME, West RF, Stanovich KE. Practitioner review: Do performance-based measures and ratings of executive function assess the same construct? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2013;54:131-43. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12001.  Back to cited text no. 38
    
39.
Barkley RA, Fischer M. Predicting impairment in major life activities and occupational functioning in hyperactive children as adults: Self-reported executive function (EF) deficits versus EF tests. Dev Neuropsychol 2011;36:137-61. doi: 10.1080/87565641.2010.549877.  Back to cited text no. 39
    
40.
Rhodes SM, Coghill DR, Matthews K. Methylphenidate restores visual memory, but not working memory function in attention deficit-hyperkinetic disorder. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 2004;175:319-30. doi: 10.1007/s00213-004-1833-7.  Back to cited text no. 40
    
41.
Rhodes SM, Coghill DR, Matthews K. Neuropsychological functioning in stimulant-naive boys with hyperkinetic disorder. Psychol Med 2005;35:1109-20.  Back to cited text no. 41
    
42.
Huang F, Sun L, Qian Y, Liu L, Ma QG, Yang L, et al. Cognitive Function of Children and Adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder and Learning Difficulties: A Developmental Perspective. Chin Med J 2016;129:1922-8. doi: 10.4103/0366-6999.187861.  Back to cited text no. 42
[PUBMED]  [Full text]  
43.
Rapport MD, Scanlan SW, Denney CB. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and scholastic achievement: A model of dual developmental pathways. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1999;40:1169-83. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00534.  Back to cited text no. 43
    
44.
Alloway TP, Gathercole SE, Elliott J. Examining the link between working memory behaviour and academic attainment in children with ADHD. Dev Med Child Neurol 2010;52:632-6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2009.03603.x.  Back to cited text no. 44
    
45.
Gropper RJ, Tannock R. A pilot study of working memory and academic achievement in college students with ADHD. J Atten Disord 2009;12:574-81. doi: 10.1177/1087054708320390.  Back to cited text no. 45
    
46.
Holmes J, Gathercole SE, Dunning DL. Adaptive training leads to sustained enhancement of poor working memory in children. Dev Sci 2009;12:F9-15. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00848.x.  Back to cited text no. 46
    
47.
Sonuga-Barke EJ, Brandeis D, Cortese S, Daley D, Ferrin M, Holtmann M, et al. Nonpharmacological interventions for ADHD: Systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials of dietary and psychological treatments. Am J Psychiatry 2013;170:275-89. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12070991.  Back to cited text no. 47
    



 
 
    Tables

  [Table 1], [Table 2], [Table 3], [Table 4], [Table 5]



 

Top
 
 
  Search
 
Similar in PUBMED
   Search Pubmed for
   Search in Google Scholar for
 Related articles
Access Statistics
Email Alert *
Add to My List *
* Registration required (free)

 
  In this article
Abstract
Introduction
Methods
Results
Discussion
References
Article Tables

 Article Access Statistics
    Viewed678    
    Printed15    
    Emailed0    
    PDF Downloaded271    
    Comments [Add]    

Recommend this journal




京ICP备05052599号